Relationship Between Dual-task Walking and Cognitive Functions in Persons with Multiple Sclerosis
PDF
Cite
Share
Request
RESEARCH ARTICLE
VOLUME: 2 ISSUE: 3
P: 57 - 61
December 2022

Relationship Between Dual-task Walking and Cognitive Functions in Persons with Multiple Sclerosis

J Mult Scler Res 2022;2(3):57-61
1. Dokuz Eylul University, Graduate School of Health Sciences, Izmir, Turkey
2. Izmir Katip Celebi University Faculty of Health Sciences, Department of Physiotherapy and Rehabilitation, Izmir, Turkey
3. Izmir Katip Celebi University, Graduate School of Health Sciences, Department of Physiotherapy and Rehabilitation, Izmir, Turkey
4. Dokuz Eylul University Faculty of Medicine, Department of Neurology, Izmir, Turkey
No information available.
No information available
Received Date: 30.07.2022
Accepted Date: 18.09.2022
PDF
Cite
Share
Request

Abstract

Objective: Dual-task performance assessment is a holistic approach that incorporates both motor and cognitive assessment. However, there is scarce data on the relationship between dual-task walking and cognitive functions in persons with multiple sclerosis (pwMS). The aim was to investigate the relationship between dual-task walking and cognitive functions in pwMS.

Materials and Methods: This study analyzed 156 patients (median age 35 years, 73.1% female). Timed Up and Go tests (TUG), with and without cognitive task (TUG), were performed to assess dual-task performance. Dual-task cost (DTC) was calculated. Cognitive information processing speed, visuospatial memory, and verbal memory were assessed using a Brief International Cognitive Assessment for MS (BICAMS).

Results: The DTC was 11.8%. The TUG-cog tests were moderately correlated with all subtests of BICAMS (r=-0.322 to -0.440). However, DTC has a significant but small correlation with cognitive tests (r=0.227-0.254). Disability level was the significant predictor of dual-task performance.

Conclusion: Our findings confirm that higher dual-task performance is significantly associated with better cognitive processing speed, visuospatial memory, and verbal memory in pwMS. This result may facilitate the use of dual-tasking paradigms in studies on cognitive impairment screening methods. However, such research undertakings should be supported by longitudinal studies.

Keywords:
Multiple sclerosis, cognition, dual-task, cognitive-motor interference, walking

Introduction

Multiple sclerosis (MS) is a neurodegenerative condition of the central nervous system, mainly characterized by walking and cognitive impairment that begin in the early stages of the disease (1). Traditionally, these symptoms are evaluated and treated separately. However, when persons with MS (pwMS) are subjected to simultaneous evaluation for motor and cognitive performance, they often experience worsening in one or both tasks (2, 3). This deterioration has been termed cognitive-motor interference (CMI), which can be quantified by calculating the percentage change between single-task and dual-task performance (4, 5).

In recent years, there has been increasing interest in the clinical characteristics of CMI, its neural correlates and associated factors in pwMS, given that it has been considered a marker of daily life impairment. Although some neuroimaging studies suggest that there is increased activation in the prefrontal cortex and premotor cortex during dual tasking in pwMS, findings on whether pwMS have higher CMI than age-matched healthy controls are inconsistent (6-8). This may be due to the fact that varied dual-task paradigms have been used in studies or different baseline conditions of patients associated with diseases. Recently, Rooney et al. (9) summarized the findings in the literature on related clinical factors of dual-task in pwMS. They found that there were few studies on the relationship between dual-task and cognition, and that of these limited studies, majority focused on the correlation between processing speed and dual-task performance. Furthermore, the results arrived at in the studies were inconsistent  (9).

In this study, we aimed to investigate the association between cognitive-motor dual-task performance, cognitive functions and falls in pwMS using a larger sample than in the studies found in the literature. In addition, we aimed to explore the relationship between different domains of cognitive functions using an extensive cognitive battery.

Materials and Methods

The Dokuz Eylul University Ethics Board approved the study protocol (approval number: 2016/27-08, date: 20.10.2016). All participants provided their written consent after being fully informed.

Participants

The data were secondary outcomes of our previously published study (10). The inclusion criteria were diagnosis of MS according to the 2017 McDonald criteria (11), Expanded Disability Status Scale (EDSS) range was between 0 and 6.5, and age was between 18 and 65 years. Exclusion criteria were the following: relapse occurring within 30 days, neurological disease diagnosis other than MS, and severe cognitive impairment according to clinician judgments as to hindered understanding of test instructions.

Outcome Measures

Primary Outcome Measures

Dual-task Performance

The Timed Up and Go (TUG) test was performed with and without a cognitive task. The subject was instructed to stand up, walk three meters to a particular spot on the ground, turn around, and go back to the chair and sit (12). The activity, timed using a stopwatch, was ended when the participant was already sitting in the chair, with the total duration was noted. Each participant underwent the TUG test in the same standardized order, and they were instructed to use their regular mobility device and walk quickly but as safely as possible. Secondly, participants performed another TUG, this time with a cognitive task (i.e., TUG-cog). The cognitive task was serial subtraction by threes from a given starting number (between 20 and 100). Participants were instructed to execute both tasks at their best without prioritization. We reported single-task performance (TUG), absolute dual-task performance (TUG-cog), and dual-task cost (DTC). DTC was calculated by this formula:

DTC (%)=[(single-task performance - dual-task performance)/(single-task performance)]x100

The larger the minus value of the DTC, the higher is the DTC, meaning, worse dual-task performance.

Cognitive Functions

The Turkish version of the Brief International Cognitive Assessment for MS (BICAMS) was administered to measure cognitive functions. It includes three subtests: the oral version of the Symbol Digit Modalities Test (SDMT) that measures cognitive processing speed and sustained attention; the Brief Visuospatial Memory Test-Revised (BVMT-R) that assesses visuospatial memory; and the California Verbal Learning Test (CVLT-II) that assesses verbal memory (13).

Secondary Outcome Measures

Demographic and clinical information such as age, sex, disease duration, course of the disease and neurological disability level rated by EDSS were noted.

Timed-25 Foot Walking (T25FW) was used to assess gait speed on 7.62 m pathway (14). The perceived walking performance was evaluated using the 12-item MS Walking Scale
(MSWS-12) (15).

Statistical Analysis

In order to analyze the data, IBM SPSS Statistics for Windows was used (Version 25.0. Armonk, NY: IBM Corp.). The Kolmogorov-Smirnov test and evaluation of the histogram and plots were utilized to determine the distribution of the data. The use of descriptive statistics yielded median and interquartile ranges since data did not show normal distribution. Spearman’s rank correlation was calculated to determine the association between absolute dual-task performance, DTC and cognitive functions, and falls. Correlation coefficients between 0.1 and 0.29 were considered to be small, 0.3-0.49 to be moderate, and 0.5-1.0 to be strong (16). Hierarchical binary regression models were conducted to explain the relationship between cognitive functions and dual-task performance.

Results

Study Participants

In total, data from 156 subjects were analyzed in this study. The median EDSS score was 1.5. For majority of the subjects, the course of the disease was relapsing-remitting. The baseline demographics, clinical characteristics, and descriptives of outcome measures of participants are summarized in Table 1.

When the TUG was applied with the cognitive task, the duration increased from 6.78 to 7.69. The median DTC was 11.82%.

Correlations of Dual-task Performance with Cognitive Functions

Based on Spearman correlation analysis, absolute dual-task performance (i.e., TUG-cog) moderately correlated with all subtests of BICAMS (r=-0.322 to -0.440). However, DTC has a significant but small correlation with cognitive tests (r=0.227-0.254). Age and EDSS were strongly correlated with both TUG and TUG-cog. Age was weakly correlated with DTC but EDSS
was found not to be correlated with DTC. Correlation coefficients are shown in Table 2.

Regression Analysis

Table 3 presents the hierarchical binary regression models to show the impact of age, disability level, and cognitive functions on absolute dual-task performance and DTC. In step 1, age and EDSS were entered, showing EDSS (β=0.58) to be significantly correlated with the TUG-cog (R2 =0.41). Step 2 included cognitive test outcomes in addition to age and EDSS, which yielded EDSS as the only variable significantly correlating with the TUG-cog that, in turn, explained 43% of the variance. The addition of cognitive tests explained an additional 2% of variance over age and disability level assessed by EDSS. For the DTC, no variable was found significant based on step 1 and step 2.

Discussion

This study aimed to investigate the relationship between cognitive functions and dual-task walking performance in pwMS. Our findings suggest that (i) pwMS showed significant CMI (DTC: -11.8%); (ii) there was a statistically significant correlation between cognitive tests and dual-task performance; and (iii) disability level assessed by EDSS was the only significant determinant factor on absolute dual-task performance in pwMS.

Previous studies that investigated the relationship between dual-task performance and cognitive functions the mechanism underlying CMI in pwMS (9, 17). Our study found that absolute dual-task performance was moderately correlated, while motor DTC was insignificantly correlated, with cognitive functions. Our results are consistent with the findings of Prosperini et al. (18) who assessed dual-task performance during static postural control task with Stroop task. Prosperini et al. (18) also found a higher correlation between SDMT and absolute dual-task performance than DTC (r=-0.481 and -0.242, respectively). In another study, Motl et al. (19) did not find a correlation with all parameters of gait but they found a significant correlation between SDMT and DTC of speed (r=-0.32). In their study, verbal fluency task was applied during a short walking distance test (19). Recently, Veldkamp et al. (20) assessed the relationship between cognition and dual-task performance with different cognitive and motor task combinations. They observed that SDMT was a factor associated with dual-task performance in less challenging walking conditions (20). Some studies do not support findings of a significant relationship between SDMT and dual-task performance, though (21, 22). However, none of these studies employed a motor test containing functional mobility tasks (i.e., TUG) as we used in this study. Despite the methodological heterogeneity in the literature, the association of better processing speed with higher dual-task performance is increasingly supported by investigations on the relationship between dual-task performance and cognitive functioning. However, further studies are needed to confirm these findings.

Our findings show significant association between disability level and absolute dual-task performance but not DTC. In the systematic review of Rooney et al. (9), no correlation between DTC and EDSS was found in most of the studies. However, due to the methodological heterogeneity of the studies, no firm conclusions could be drawn about the relationship between disability level and dual-task performance in pwMS. Nonetheless, some studies show that a higher disability level is associated with lower dual-task performance (19, 23) but such findings should be confirmed by future studies involving participants having different disability levels and the use of different motor-cognitive tasks.

We found a correlation between dual-task performance in different domains of cognition with similar magnitude. To the best of our knowledge, there is no other study that examined this relationship using the BICAMS battery, which is valid for pwMS and includes different cognitive domains. This study showed that in addition to cognitive processing speed, verbal memory and visuospatial memory are also associated with dual-task performance in pwMS.

Study Limitations

Although our study includes a relatively larger sample size compared to studies found in the literature, some limitations should be noted. Firstly, we mostly included patients with mild disability, which affects the generalizability of our results to persons with moderate and severe disability. Secondly, we evaluated dual-task performance using the TUG test with a cognitive task. Although the evaluation is reflective of daily life functionality as it includes many activities that are undertaken in everyday living, such as sitting, walking, and turning, the limitation is that it is a short-term test. Additionally, there is no test-retest reliability study on TUG with the cognitive task in pwMS. Furthermore, since its reliability is low, we did not assess cognitive DTC. Lastly, we also did not assess gait parameters by instrumented gait analysis methods. In future studies, we recommend measuring the dual-task performance during different TUG tasks with the use of wearable sensors.

Conclusion

Our results confirm that higher dual-task performance is significantly associated with better cognitive processing speed, visuospatial memory, and verbal memory in pwMS. This finding may facilitate the use of dual-tasking paradigms in research on cognitive impairment screening methods. However, such investigations should be supported by longitudinal studies.

Ethics

Ethics Committee Approval: The Dokuz Eylul University Ethics Board approved the study protocol (approval number: 2016/27-08, date: 20.10.2016).
Informed Consent: All participants provided their written consent after being fully informed.
Peer-review: Internally peer-reviewed.

Authorship Contributions

Surgical and Medical Practices: S.O., Concept: Z.A., P.Y., H.K., S.O., Design: Z.A., P.Y., H.K., S.O., Data Collection or Processing: Z.A., P.Y., H.K., S.O., Analysis or Interpretation: Z.A., P.Y., H.K., S.O., Literature Search: Z.A., P.Y., H.K., S.O., Writing: Z.A.
Conflict of Interest: No conflict of interest was declared by the authors.
Financial Disclosure: The authors declared that this study received no financial support.

References

1
Compston A, Coles A. Multiple sclerosis. The Lancet 2008:372:1502-1517.
2
Leone C, Patti F, Feys P. Measuring the cost of cognitive-motor dual tasking during walking in multiple sclerosis. Mult Scler 2015;21:123-131.
3
Wajda DA, Sosnoff JJ. Cognitive-motor interference in multiple sclerosis: A systematic review of evidence, correlates, and consequences. Biomed Res Int 2015;2015:720856.
4
Plummer P, Eskes G, Wallace S, Giuffrida C, Fraas M, Campbell G, Clifton KL, Skidmore ER; American Congress of Rehabilitation Medicine Stroke Networking Group Cognition Task Force. Cognitive-motor interference during functional mobility after stroke: state of the science and implications for future research. Arch Phys Med Rehabil 2013;94:2565-2574.e6.
5
McIsaac TL, Lamberg EM, Muratori LM. Building a framework for a dual task taxonomy. Biomed Res Int 2015;2015:591475.
6
Veldkamp R, Goetschalckx M, Hulst HE, Nieuwboer A, Grieten K, Baert I, Leone C, Moumdjian L, Feys P. Cognitive-motor Interference in Individuals With a Neurologic Disorder: A Systematic Review of Neural Correlates. Cogn Behav Neurol 2021;34:79-95.
7
Kim H, Fraser S. Neural correlates of dual-task walking in people with central neurological disorders: a systematic review. J Neurol 2022;269:2378-2402.
8
Learmonth YC, Ensari I, Motl RW. Cognitive Motor Interference in Multiple Sclerosis: Insights From a Systematic Quantitative Review. Arch Phys Med Rehabil 2017;98:1229-1240.
9
Rooney S, Ozkul C, Paul L. Correlates of dual-task performance in people with multiple sclerosis: A systematic review. Gait Posture 2020;81:172-182.
10
Abasıyanık Z, Kahraman T, Yigit P, Baba C, Ertekin O, Ozakbas S. Further Validity of the Short Version of the Activities-Specific Balance Confidence Scale in Patients with Multiple Sclerosis. Journal of Multiple Sclerosis Research 2021;1:7-12.
11
Thompson AJ, Banwell BL, Barkhof F, Carroll WM, Coetzee T, Comi G, Correale J, Fazekas F, Filippi M, Freedman MS, Fujihara K, Galetta SL, Hartung HP, Kappos L, Lublin FD, Marrie RA, Miller AE, Miller DH, Montalban X, Mowry EM, Sorensen PS, Tintoré M, Traboulsee AL, Trojano M, Uitdehaag BMJ, Vukusic S, Waubant E, Weinshenker BG, Reingold SC, Cohen JA. Diagnosis of multiple sclerosis: 2017 revisions of the McDonald criteria. Lancet Neurol 2018;17:162-173.
12
Sebastião E, Sandroff BM, Learmonth YC, Motl RW. Validity of the Timed Up and Go Test as a Measure of Functional Mobility in Persons With Multiple Sclerosis. Arch Phys Med Rehabil 2016;97:1072-1077.
13
Ozakbas S, Yigit P, Cinar BP, Limoncu H, Kahraman T, Kösehasanogullari G. The Turkish validation of the Brief International Cognitive Assessment for Multiple Sclerosis (BICAMS) battery. BMC Neurol 2017;17:208.
14
Fischer JS, Rudick RA, Cutter GR, Reingold SC. The Multiple Sclerosis Functional Composite Measure (MSFC): an integrated approach to MS clinical outcome assessment. National MS Society Clinical Outcomes Assessment Task Force. Mult Scler 1999;5:244-250.
15
Dib H, Tamam Y, Terzi M, Hobart J. Testing patient-reported outcome measurement equivalence in multinational clinical trials: An exemplar using the 12-item Multiple Sclerosis Walking Scale. Mult Scler J Exp Transl Clin 2017;3:205521731772874.
16
Hillside NJ. Statistical power analysis for the behavioural sciences.[Google Scholar]. Statistical Power Anaylsis for the Behavioral Sciences. 1988.
17
Baldasso BD, Kirkland MC, Newell CJ, Ploughman M. Better Cognitive Function Predicts Maintenance of Dual-Task Walking Ability Over Time Among People With Relapsing-Remitting MS. Neuropsychology 2022;36:520-527.
18
Prosperini L, Castelli L, Sellitto G, De Luca F, De Giglio L, Gurreri F, Pozzilli C. Investigating the phenomenon of “cognitive-motor interference” in multiple sclerosis by means of dual-task posturography. Gait Posture 2015;41:780-785.
19
Motl RW, Sosnoff JJ, Dlugonski D, Pilutti LA, Klaren R, Sandroff BM. Walking and cognition, but not symptoms, correlate with dual task cost of walking in multiple sclerosis. Gait Posture 2014;39:870-874.
20
Veldkamp R, Baert I, Kalron A, Romberg A, Tacchino A, Giffroy X, Coninx K, Feys P. Associations between clinical characteristics and dual task performance in Multiple Sclerosis depend on the cognitive and motor dual tasks used. Mult Scler Relat Disord 2021;56:103230.
21
Sandroff BM, Benedict RH, Motl RW. Nonsignificant Associations between Measures of Inhibitory Control and Walking while Thinking in Persons with Multiple Sclerosis. Arch Phys Med Rehabil 2015;96:1518-1524.
22
Sosnoff JJ, Socie MJ, Sandroff BM, Balantrapu S, Suh Y, Pula JH, Motl RW. Mobility and cognitive correlates of dual task cost of walking in persons with multiple sclerosis. Disabil Rehabil 2014;36:205-209.
23
Butchard-Macdonald E, Paul L, Evans JJ. Balancing the Demands of Two Tasks: An Investigation of Cognitive-Motor Dual-Tasking in Relapsing Remitting Multiple Sclerosis. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 2018;24:247-258.